|
Volume 78 1969 > Volume 78, No. 2 > Kwaio word tabooing in its cultural context, by Roger M. Keesing and Jonathan Fifii, p 154 - 177
|
![]() | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
- 154
![]() KWAIO WORD TABOOING IN ITS CULTURAL CONTEXT
University of California, Santa Cruz Scattered through the dictionaries and grammars of Oceanic languages are fragmentary references to words tabooed because of their association with things sacred or proscribed. Although word tabooing seems to have been widespread, and some cases (e.g. Tahiti) are fairly well known to specialists, the phenomenon has received little comparative study. The distributions of word tabooing in the Pacific have not been worked out. For those cases described in the literature, evidence is usually meager. It is seldom possible to estimate the frequency of tabooing or the types of forms selected; the mechanisms whereby particular forms were selected for tabooing are not known in detail; and there is seldom much evidence - 155 Yet at this point it seems possible that word tabooing was characteristic of some or all early Austronesian speakers in the Pacific. That possibility, in turn, has profound implications for comparative study of Oceanic languages and the use of linguistic evidence in reconstructing Pacific prehistory. Why is the evidence so meager? Largely because the very forces that led to the occasional recording of word tabooing were leading at the same time to its disappearance. Evidence is needed of word tabooing as an ongoing process, not an ancient custom; yet in most of the island Pacific that process came to a halt decades ago. The potential importance of word tabooing in Oceanic linguistics derives from the possibility that this process has significantly accelerated vocabulary differentiation between genetically related languages. To the extent that “basic vocabulary” forms were tabooed, lexicostatistical analyses of Austronesian languages could be affected. Such tabooing could create a spurious impression of long divergence or skew datings, or in some cases could even hide genetic connections. 3 If tabooing affected less common, or “cultural”, forms, leaving a basic vocabulary largely untouched, this could create an impression of a layer of common Austronesian forms superimposed on a substratum of genetically unrelated languages. To illustrate such possibilities, let me simply note three problems in current Oceanic linguistics: first, the problems of reconciling retention rate constants of glottochronology with archeological time scales; 4 second, the greater lexical differentiation in Melanesian languages than grammatical structure and other evidence would lead us to expect; 5 and third, substratum and pidginisation theories of Melanesian languages. 6 As a social anthropologist, not a linguist, I must leave to others the comparative study of word tabooing in Austronesian languages and assessment of the implications of word tabooing for subgrouping of Oceanic languages. However, my recent field work among the pagan Kwaio of Malaita, British Solomon Islands, among whom word tabooing is still very much an ongoing process, gives me an opportunity to supplement the present meager evidence on word tabooing, and particularly to examine it as a social phenomenon. THE KWAIO OF MALAITA
About 6,000 Kwaio speakers inhabit the mountainous central area of Malaita. Kwaio is one of some ten Melanesian languages spoken in belts across the 100-mile-long island. Kwara ? ae and ? Are? are, spoken in the adjoining zones, are almost completely unintelligible to Kwaio speakers. No adequate comparative analyses of Malaita languages, or attempts at sub-grouping, have yet been carried out. In pre-European times, the Kwaio were a “bush” people, growing taro - 156 KWAIO SOCIAL STRUCTURE AND RELIGION
Word tabooing is a reflection of Kwaio beliefs about the sacred, and hence to comprehend it one must begin with an outline of Kwaio religion. Further, since the restrictions on linguistic usage apply to particular actors on the social scene, they can be interpreted only within the framework of Kwaio social structure. To separate “religion” from “social structure” distorts Kwaio conceptualisation of their social universe which is populated with both human actors and their ‘ancestors’ (adalo). 8 These ancestral spirits, as unseen actors on the social stage, enter almost every phase of Kwaio life. Social relations of the living are often defined and ordered in terms of ancestors. Thus in sketching the “social” and the “religious”, an attempt will be made to show as well their interdependence and articulation. The Kwaio landscape is carved into small territories composed of a patchwork of land tracts conceptually grouped under a focal shrine. Each territory is, or once was, the spatial locus of a descent group or lineage defined in terms of descent from the ancestors founding the territory. Relations with descent group ancestors are mediated by a priest at the focal shrine and various subsidiary shrines and at the sacred men's house he maintains. Recruitment to these lineages is normally patrilateral, reflecting a strong ideology of agnation. However, a strong secondary interest in the maternal group can be converted to a primary affiliation by long residence, especially in childhood. Since in each generation double, if asymmetrical, relationships and rights are traced through each parent, the cumulative result is a wide cognatic extension of secondary land rights and relations to ancestors. Looked at another way, each individual acquires, in addition - 157 This combination of discrete descent groups and wide-spread cognatic ties to lands and ancestors has a paradoxical result. On the one hand, each descent group comprises a separate land-owning corporation and above all a ritual community linked to common ancestors. But on the other, neither land rights nor ritual relationships are exclusively exercised by group members: a cluster of outsiders cognatically descended from the founding ancestors, but primarily affiliated elsewhere, have secondary interests in the territory and its ancestors. From ancestors comes the “power” (nanamaŋaa, metathesised and nominalised “mana”) whereby the living attain success, “good living”, successful gardening, acquisition of wealth, successful fighting, etc. Ancestors convey this power when they are pleased with the living; they are pleased when their descendants follow proper ritual procedures, observe taboos (especially those pertaining to the ritual uncleanness of women), and raise propitiatory pigs consecrated to particular ancestors. When ancestors are displeased, especially as a result of violation of a taboo, they cause misfortune, sickness, and death. Which ancestor is causing misfortune, why, and what can be done about it (usually sacrifice of expiatory pigs) is determined by divination. Ancestors vary in the degree to which they are ‘sacred’ (abu, Oceanic “tapu”). Just as living Kwaio exemplify the “big man” system of Melanesia, so ancestors are ordered in terms of their power and influence within a group of related ancestors. Each descent group might be thought of as joined with what I have elsewhere called an “ancestral kin group”. Within this “group”, one (or sometimes two) ancestors will be most powerful. Sacrifices to the entire “group” are usually made through this “leader”. The process whereby an adalo rises to prominence can only be inferred by extrapolation. Individuals who were prominent in life as major priests, successful fighters, important feast givers, etc. 9 are “important dead” when they depart the ranks of the living. As such, their death leads to elaborate ritual observances. Such a deceased person has a promising start toward being a powerful adalo: but as in life, only a start. How powerful he becomes depends not only on the attribution of special power to him, which reflects the power he had in life, but also the outcome of divination and the success of his descendants. The rise to power of an adalo is in practice also contingent on the proliferation of his descendants. The most powerful adalo appear on genealogies some twelve to fifteen generations back. Through the proliferation and geographical spread of their descendants (particularly through out-marriage of women), they have come to be venerated at a number of shrines by many different descent groups, through different priests. The few most powerful adalo are claimed as ancestors by more than half of the people within a wide - 158 To provide sufficient background for a discussion on word tabooing, it is necessary to go briefly into some detail on the fo?ota system. The propitiatory pigs consecrated to an ancestor, are called fo?ota (‘offering’). An adalo is given a special name, a kind of pseudonym, that is used for the consecrated pigs, and because the original name is sacred this fo?ota name comes to be used for the adalo in lieu of the name used in life. In the case of the most powerful ancestors, the fo?ota name may come to refer not to a single ancestor but to a family cluster of ancestors who distinguished themselves in life by collective action; for example, father and his sons, a set of brothers, or a mother and her daughter. A pig bearing the fo?ota name is consecrated to them as a unit to elicit the power they convey collectively. The rise of an adalo to sacredness is reflected in the generation span between death and the time fo?ota pigs raised for the adalo become so sacred that only adult male descendants can eat them. Before this time, the adalo is minor and both ancestor and consecrated pigs are only minimally sacred. A sacred priest becomes sacred to his grandchildren; a ritually adult man becomes sacred to his great-grandchildren; others become sacred, if at all, to their great-great-grandchildren. A minor adalo, usually the parent or grandparent of the person sacrificing to it, is important for sentimental reasons and as an intermediary to more distant and powerful ancestors. An individual's ritual position is defined largely by the fo?ota pigs he raises. An adult man usually “keeps” from eighteen to fifteen fo?ota to which he may periodically consecrate pigs, though at any one time he is likely to be raising pigs consecrated to only some four or five. When the consecrated pig is fully grown, it will be sacrificed to the ancestor by the priest and will be ritually eaten by the adult men descended from that ancestor. In sociological terms, descent group members will share a common cluster of descent group ancestors and partake of these sacrifices together, augmented by some cognatic descendants of these ancestors affiliated elsewhere. Descent group members are also related to different ancestors through their mothers, mothers' mothers, fathers' mothers, and so on, that differentiate them ritually. These non-agnatic ancestors, and sacrifice to them, provide a ritual expression of genealogical segmentation within a descent group. Thus, an ancestor traced through a man's FaFaFaMo will be common to a whole descent group segment; an ancestor traced through his FaMo will be common to a much smaller segment. Finally, in the case of those adalo that have come to be common to large segments of Kwaio society and are propitiated at different shrines by different priests, the sacrifice of large fo?ota pigs at mortuary feasts provides an occasion for men of these larger descent categories to express their unity by sacred commensality. 10 A final principle regarding adalo is also necessary to an understanding - 159 WHY WORDS BECOME TABOO
A. Principles of Tabooing. The name of a person, in Kwaio culture, is associated with the ‘essence’ (to ?ofuŋana) of that person. The name of an adalo thus acquires sacredness roughly corresponding to the sacredness of the adalo.
There are several permutations on this basic principle. The first two reflect the attribution of extreme sacredness to an adalo.
A fifth principle reflects the association between a name and the person who bears it, but not extreme sacredness of the adaio with that name.
Two subsidiary principles—only the first involving association of a name with a linguistic form—reflect the dangerous sacredness of two feared diseases, leprosy and tuberculosis. - 160![]()
A final mechanism is recent:
B. Kwaio Naming. Before examining the associations between names and linguistic forms that lead to tabooing something should be said about Kwaio names. Most Kwaio names comprise two word bases, sometimes connected by particles. The most common form is a verbal base plus a noun base (usually corresponding to an English transitive verb plus direct object): “hit corpse”, “spear fish”, “drink dew”, “string money”. Less common are two noun bases connected by particles that in common usage of the name are omitted (“money-shrine”, which might be “money for the shrine”); an intransitive verbal base plus noun base, connected by particles in the full form (“look-pig”, for “look for the pig”); a single noun base (“panpipes”); a combination of noun bases; or some other grammatically possible combination. Originally these names are based on some event in the recipient's early life, but they are often passed down through several generations. An individual receives at least two names in life, one for childhood and a second for adulthood. However, many people go through several names in a lifetime and have two or even three in use at a given time. A frequent pattern is abbreviation of a name. Usually, for a man or a woman, the abbreviation is {-xa}, added to the first word base: e.g. Go →ubisu, ‘drink dew’, →Go →uxa; ?Ai ?aniaba ?e, ‘insist about shrine’,→Aixa. Other particles are rarely used, particularly {-ta}, e.g. Galuoŋa →Galuta. Some contradictions might seem to exist between the sacredness of an ancestral name (or the sentiment connected with the name of a deceased kinsman) and the passing of a name from generation to generation. Four points bear mention in this connection: (1) Acquiring the name of a deceased relative, in a sense, reactivates his status or continues that essence associated with the name, and hence is complementary to tabooing of the name 12 in expressing esteem or respect for the dead. (2) Such sequences of names are usually given in that span of generations—parental to great-grandparental—where deceased persons are not yet - 161 fo?ota names are generally similar, except that they usually involve some incident or characteristic of a deceased person's later life. They consist somewhat less often of a single noun base. Finally it should be noted that there are some word bases, mainly noun bases, that are extremely common in names. This, as will be seen, affects the working out of word tabooing mechanisms. There follows a calculation of the percentage of incidence of some common forms used in men's and women's names, from a sample of 3681 names.
The interesting aspects to note from the table are two: 1) that Kwaio name segments seem to be asexual in their incidence of occurrence; and 2) that these ten common name segments order themselves into four fairly tight groups. C. Linguistic Associations Leading to Tabooing. Here will be analysed the purely linguistic associations between names and linguistic forms that cause the latter to be tabooed. In the next section, the factors will be examined that actually precipitate or inhibit tabooing of a form. (1) A name is a word base. The simplest and most direct relationship between name and language form occurs when the name consists of a single word base (sometimes with bound particle). This can occur with either an adalo name or a fo?ota name. Examples: - 162![]()
(2) A name is a modified form of a word base. Here, the base is manifest in the name in some morphologically modified shape (often a reduplicated form). In this case, the base itself is usually tabooed, though not necessarily in all the constructions it enters into. The derivative form in the adalo's name is also tabooed.
(3) A name is one of two component bases in a compound form, and another base is substituted for it. Here the adalo name could be homonymous with the segment of the compound form. Example: Gola, an adalo name, led to tabooing of /madagola/, ‘hot coals’, and its replacement by /madalafa/. (4) The name contains two bases, one of which is then tabooed. Here. one must account for the tabooing of one of the bases, but not the other. Examples:
(5) A form is tabooed because of phonological similarity to a name. Most often, here, the name is not morphologically related to the tabooed form. Examples: - 163![]()
(6) A name consisting of two bases may be tabooed so that either base can be used separately, but they cannot be conjoined. In this construction, one form could not be used, and some substitute would be required; or some separating “buffer” would be introduced. Examples:
(7) A name is a morphological modification of a word base, and that morphologically complex form is tabooed while the base and other derivative forms are not.
WHAT WORDS BECOME TABOO
Since most names, or fo?ota names, contain one or more word bases—or could otherwise lead to tabooing of a linguistic form according to the principles outlined above—the problem here is why some names lead to word tabooing but most do not. This depends on a number of interacting factors.
![]()
![]()
To return to the centrally important question, in terms of Austronesian linguistics, of a possible taboo-resistant “core vocabulary”, the effects can now be examined of Kwaio word tabooing among the 100 items on Swadesh's revised lexicostatistic item list, grouped according to his semantic categories. Table 2 shows the frequency, in each category, with which (A) the Kwaio evidence from the Sinalagu area shows a form to have been tabooed for at least some people; (B) the form has been tabooed only in some particular construction; and (C) there is no evidence that the form has been tabooed. TABLE 2
Of the twelve tabooed words, two—/manu/, ‘bird’, and/mela/, ‘red’—are tabooed throughout the entire East Coast of Kwaio territory. It is - 166 In summary, if the name of an adalo whose sacredness is increasing is associated directly with a linguistic form not common in names or highly frequent in usage, for which some substitute form is available, and the form has no ritual associations and is not part of a “core” vocabulary, then chances of tabooing are very strong. As these factors are reversed, the probability of tabooing drops markedly. WHAT WORDS REPLACE THEM?
When a linguistic form is tabooed, how is a replacement form derived? The institutionalisation of word tabooing in Kwaio culture appears to produce and require the availability in the language of far more alternative monolexemic labels for the same cultural segregates, 16 i.e. “synonyms”, than any other language with which I am familiar. For “bamboo fire-tongs” there are five alternative lexemes. For “pudding bowl” there are four, for “leaf stopper for cooking bamboo” there are three. These almost random examples of alternative lexemes for cultural objects correctly suggest that when the decision to taboo a word base or construction is made, potential replacement forms are very often known and available. This, however, only pushes the problem a step further back. How are new forms produced in the first place? How is it that new forms subunia ‘sprout up’? In this section, I will outline those processes I have been able to isolate. (1) Productive morphological devices generate from a semantically related word base, a semantically equivalent form. Examples:
![]() (2) Semantic generalisation from a narrower form. Frequently this elevates to a higher level of a taxonomy, a form previously used only in a lower included contrast set. 17 Example:
(3) Semantic shift of a fairly radical sort (though based on some analogy, or a common derivative or metaphorical meaning). Examples:
(4) Use, in a compound form, of a substitute word base with some semantic overlap or resemblance. Examples:
(5) A loan word is borrowed from another Malaita language or dialect. Examples:
(6) A word is borrowed from Pidgin English.
(7) A new term is coined that modifies an existing form phonologically. - 168![]()
(8) A descriptive phrase or compound form is created. These may be semantically endocentric or exocentric. 18
(9) An alternative productive morphological device is used to create a semantically equivalent construction.
(10 A radically new term is coined. A full compilation of tabooed Kwaio terms, the reasons for taboo, and replacement forms will shortly be published. This compilation will be keyed to the present analysis, so that the materials presented here for illustration can be viewed against the full range of Kwaio data. WORD TABOOING AS A SOCIAL PROCESS
The reader is asked to return, now, to thinking about the Kwaio social scene. Recall the descent groups, clustered around territories; the skeins of cognatic kinship linking outsiders to these descent groups; and the chains of cognatic descent linking groups to common ancient ancestors. Place in this landscape the tabooing of speech forms so they cannot be used by, or in the presence of, descendants of ancestors with the associated names. A host of new questions arises. How do people know what forms they cannot use, and with whom? How are the taboos first imposed? What restrictions, exactly, are placed on usage? What happens when a rule is violated? How, in short, are the abstract principles of word tabooing worked out in concrete social situations in such a way that communication is maintained? 1. Knowledge about Taboos. My first introduction to word tabooing came when, after five months in Kwaio, I set out to visit a distant settlement: “Don't use folia, xu ?i, or ele at Maaxona's village” I was warned. “They - 169 2. Types of Taboos. It is the right of the descendants of the ancestor from whom the taboo derives to place restrictions on usage. As already noted, these are usually thought to be placed by the ancestor himself, through divination. There are, however, several general forms of restrictions that can be placed, with some local and individual variation within each category.
![]()
3. Initial Imposition of a Taboo. It should be recalled here that the ancestor himself usually imposes the taboo by punishing the usage (causing his descendant to be sick), then communicating the taboo in divination. A usual sequence leading to tabooing would approximate the following:
One should emphasise here that while many people and descent groups have forms they themselves do not use, the number of people known to make a major issue out of usage by an outsider—especially when the descendant imposing the taboo is himself visiting another place—is much smaller. When such a person appears, others will often be warned - 171 4. Word Tabooing in Space. Because a descent group shares an array of common ancestors, its members come to share a set of tabooed forms. Thus a tabooed form is associated with a place and a kin group, and this serves as a means for remembering a taboo and to whom it applies. Two processes lead to the dispersal of a taboo beyond this group. The first process is descent group segmentation, through which genealogical segments hive off and eventually establish new territories. This is a slow, long-range process. More interesting to us, because it occurs so often, is the out-marriage of a girl from one descent group into another. When this takes place, she may introduce the taboos from her group among her affines. A girl who marries from a place with many strong taboos, or a place quite far away, can cause a considerable stir amoung her new neighbours. In the longer run, her children and grandchildren can start a new local cluster within which the taboos are observed. In the case of those ancestors common to large proportions of the population in a region, this process of out-marriage, proliferation, and out-marriage again has gone through several cycles. Within a region of some ten to twenty square miles, most Kwaio adults have a great deal of information about descent groups and territories and the cognatic relationships of particular individuals. Beyond this range, they think in terms of larger geographical categories: in the case of the Sinalagu area I studied, the main ones were Lafea, the very broken and mountainous central part of the island, Ulu, the harbour and adjacent mountains to the north-west, and ?Olobuli, the harbour and adjacent mountains to the south-east. 19 In these peripheral areas, there are significant variations in vocabulary, many of which resulted from word tabooing, and some from “normal” processes of lexical diversification. Kwaio necessarily treat the word taboos, or possible taboos inferred from differential usage, from such a peripheral area, with less efficiency. That is, they can no longer differentiate what is taboo for whom. To the extent they modify their speech at all, they do so for everyone. Until events of recent years, they seldom encountered such situations. Mortuary feasts, marriages, and markets would sometimes bring them into contact with people from afar, but interaction with them was always strained and rendered as safe as possible by magical protection against the malevolent spirits or evil magic they might bring. Adalo were too busy facing off against one another, in a game much like that of electronic countermeasures in modern strategic bombing, to worry much about word taboos. However, the Maasina Rule (“Marching Rule”) movement of the late 1940s and subsequent developments have led to regular meetings of large groups from distant areas. In the weekly “Sub-District Committee” - 172 5. Some Empirical Cases. From the many instances where word tabooing shows up in our field notes, two cases will add some empirical substance to the generalisations herein advanced. The first is interesting in that it shows a series of fairly extreme events that attracted much attention. The second is revealing as a normal and unspectacular sample of word tabooing as a regular ongoing process. The first begins with a woman named Dauli ?i, whose agnatic descent group is Talanilau, inland from Sinalagu, but who grew up with her maternal kin at Nu ?u inland from ?Olobuli. When she married a man from another Sinalagu descent group, some years ago, three forms associated with her ancestors from Nu ?u were tabooed for use in her settlement: asuaa, ‘mid-morning’, (funu) funu, ‘fight’, and fee ?ai, ‘(single) tree’. She claimed compensation when they were used in her home, but not elsewhere. Her oldest son Liŋeafaxa is now a teenager, and something of the Kwaio version of a juvenile delinquent. In early 1966, Liŋeafaxa was visiting the house of a kinsman near the coast when an elderly gentleman named Fa?atalo came into the house for a visit. Someone else then came in and asked Fa?atalo when he had left home. In his reply, he used the term asuaa (‘mid-morning’). Liŋeafaxa then announced that he had ‘sworn’ (taa) against anyone using asuaa or funu in his presence anywhere. Fa?atalo was angry that such a swear had been made unannounced—since the form of Liŋeafaxa's swear had made the usage an offence against Fa?atalo's ancestors as well as Liŋeafaxa's. The litigation and “Subdistrict Committee” investigation dragged on for some weeks, and ranged from a general discussion of the legitimacy of word tabooing to investigation of the swear. The swear was interesting. Liŋeafaxa, it turned out, had been hanging around with another wild young man (and frequent accomplice in pig theft), Laitala. Laitala, though not yet 20, had succeeded his father as “priest” at an important shrine, and sacrificed several important fo ?ota, including one called Te ?ealo. The young men had been cooking taro. Liŋeafaxa jokingly referred to the taro corm he was cooking as “te ?e alo ŋai agu”, (‘my one taro’—alo is the Kwaio reflex of proto-Eastern Oceanic *ntalo, but is usually used not for taro in general or a single plant or corm but for a unit of 100 taro corms). Laitala angrily - 173 Fa?atalo was rightfully incensed at having unwittingly offended his own ancestors, though he was prepared to give a pig as compensation to Dauli ?i's ancestors. The matter was more or less resolved, in the end, when Liŋeafaxa's father gave two pigs as compensation to Fa?atalo's ancestors and Fa?atalo gave one as compensation to Dauli ?i's ancestors. An interesting aspect of the investigation was consultation with two important priests from ?Olobuli who were much more directly descended from Asuaa and Funufunu than Dauli ?i. They said that though these names were sacred when used in the “shrine”, and they avoided using them themselves, they did not claim compensation if someone else used them: “they are words other people use all the time, and we shouldn't try to stop them.” Confronted with this, Dauli ?i and her husband asserted that “these adalo were stronger with affines than with direct descendants”—an argument that was greeted with scepticism. The second case was more routine. A girl named Dedei ?a, the daughter of an old priest and sorcerer at the edge of Lafea, married a young man named Seda from a descent group near the coast. Several forms were taboo for her father and other kin. However, it was not clear that these ancestors would enforce them in such a far-away place. But one by one, the major forms taboo for Dedei ?a's people were found in divination to be the cause of illnesses, or simply changed; thus an entire local cluster of kin began to avoid usage of several common forms not associated with their own ancestors. Here two examples will suffice. First, one of Dedei ?a's ancestors was named Laubasi (“seize bow”). Seda's FaFaBrSo was also named Laubasi. As a precaution, shortly after she came to live with them, his name was changed to Maiamae (a name he had used earlier in life). Second, the wife of one of Seda's kin visited them shortly after birth of their child (only a few months after the marriage). She used the term gwaea, ‘hold in the arms’, a form taboo for Dedei ?a's people. When the baby got sick several days later, this was divined to be the cause of illness; the “priest” sacrificed a pig in expiation, and members of the local group began to avoid use of the term. WORD TABOOING AND THE MALAITA LANGUAGES
Available information strongly suggests that the Malaitan languages comprise a single subgroup within a cluster of South-east Solomons Austronesian languages. This South-east Solomons cluster itself appears to comprise a subgroup of fairly closely related languages within the tentatively demarcated subgroup that has recently been referred to as “Eastern Oceanic”. These languages provide an ideal laboratory for intensive comparative study of “Melanesian” languages. In such a task, which I feel deserves a high priority in Oceanic linguistics, 21 full use will - 174 The occurrence of word tabooing was noted, and its effects on linguistic diversity observed, in a missionary grammar of Kwara ?ae, 22 a language spoken in the zone adjacent to Kwaio: One of the reasons for different words for the same object in contiguous dialects is that if a word in common use happens to be included in the name of an important chief or priest, when the man dies his name may become tabu; accordingly, another word has to be coined to take the place of the one thus prohibited. Take for instance the common word asi, ‘sea’; in Malu ?u the word for sea is amali because in time past asi was included in the name of an important man who died, in consequence of which it had to be dropped; but even in Malu ?u the word asi appears in their ancient songs, still sung, showing it was originally in current use. This explains many remarkable forms to be found as substitutes of words of wide use.
In the task of subgrouping and reconstruction, such diversity need not in itself be a stumbling block. Shared morphological or grammatical innovations should still be effective markers of relationship, and in languages with as many cognate forms as these, lexical diversification greater than “normal” would not in itself upset reconstruction of sound correspondences and a lexicon. A good start in this direction has been made by Richard Levy and Nathan Smith. However, one element of word tabooing, at least in Kwaio, could well lead to complications in use of the comparative method. This is the coining of replacement forms based on minor phonological modification of the original, tabooed, forms. Since such changes are apparently relatively unsystematic, they could cumulatively present to the analyst numbers of forms that appear to be cognate but do not fit regular patterns of sound correspondence. Even where no active taboos are known, Kwaio often contains two or more interchangeable terms for the same common object, act, or phenomenon, that differ only in minor phonological variation (usually consonantal shift). Many of these probably arose initially through tabooing. But whatever their origin, they help to provide a reservoir of replacement forms when these are required. Thus, in Kwaio, there exist sets like the following:
![]()
Some patterns begin to emerge here, as in the alternation of /m/ and /n/. In other cases, such as the last two, the alternative form corresponds to the cognate form in a neighbouring language (Kwara ?ae, Kwarekwareo, Laŋalaŋa, ?Are ?are), and probably derives from borrowing. Such alternative forms are apparently common in other Malaitan languages for which lexical evidence is available (To ?abaita, Lau, Kwara ?ae and Sa ?a). If such sets were characteristic of *Proto-Malaitan, the original empirical existence of doublets and triplets would make the task of reconstruction more formidable, but would render more comprehensible some of the seeming phonological irregularities that show up in Malaitan lexicons. COMPARATIVE IMPLICATIONS OF KWAIO WORD TABOOING
The Kwaio data bring home forcefully how little is known about word tabooing in the Pacific. Without evidence that is lost forever we will never be able to make really effective comparative analyses. What is known suggests that Kwaio was by no means extreme in the development of tabooing. Sociologically, Kwaio stands toward the pole of fragmentation, lack of formal political organisations and fluidity. Kwaio word - 176 The writers have reflected already on some implications for the historical linguist of the Kwaio evidence. Here some cultural-historical speculation may be useful as well. Perhaps early “Eastern Oceanic” speakers in the Pacific practised some form of word tabooing; alternatively some element in these ancestral cultures may later have given rise to word tabooing in various parts of the Pacific. It would be useful to account not only for tabooing due to association with the names of ancestors, as in Kwaio, but also for quite different principles, such as tabooing due to association with the name of a relative in a tabooed kinship category. 23 Perhaps the early Oceanic form of tabooing was quite specific, but derivative forms have simply shifted as the foci of sacredness and proscription have changed and diverged. On the other hand, the original basis of Oceanic tabooing might lie in as general a principle as a strong association between a name and the person bearing the name. This, combined with the concept of tapu, could then lead to the separate evolution of word taboos in many places where extreme tapu became attributed, for varying reasons, to named individuals. But to speculate further along these lines would be premature until such evidence as can be gathered, in the field and library, is systematically put together. This evidence will doubtless be fragmentary. Particularly in linguistics, word tabooing will probably be encounted with increasing frequency, but by indirection, as a hypothetical construct that accounts for statistics and reconstructions that do not accord with other evidence 24 or as an explanation for lexical differentiation. But to end with an optimistic note, it is possible that the recent acceleration of intensive social anthropology field work in the Pacific will turn up new evidence of word tabooing as an ongoing social and linguistic process, even in areas where it was thought such evidence could no longer be found. Finally, evidence on Austronesian word tabooing can usefully be arrayed with evidence on similar processes in other language families and other parts of the world. Regularities in linguistic and sociological process will probably emerge from such study, and they, in turn, may well prove important to other questions of cultural theory. The recent suggestions by Lévi-Strauss 25 regarding naming as a central manifestation of La Pensée Sauvage open intriguing new vistas for exploration. REFERENCES
![]()
1 Field work on Malaita in 1962-64 and 1966, was supported by the U.S. Public Health Service. Other phases of Kwaio research have been supported by the University of California, the Social Science Research Council, the Ford Foundation and the National Science Foundation. In addition to the collaboration of Mr Fifi?i, I am indebted for encouragement, suggestions, and references to the following: Bruce Biggs, Arthur Capell, William Davenport, Isidore Dyen, George Grace, Dell Hymes, and William Shipley.
2 To avoid complexity of explication, the paper is mostly written as if I (R.M.K.) were sole author. Mr Fifi?i, in addition to being a splendid informant on word taboos, produced some fifty pages of documentation and analysis of Kwaio word tabooing in a phonemic orthography of Kwaio, that provided a far richer corpus of data than I brought back from the field. His analysis was produced while he was resident for a year in Santa Cruz, supported by a Ford Foundation International-Comparative Grant through the University of California. Though distillation of his analysis and placement in sociological context were mine, his contributions are manifest throughout.
3 Hymes 1960:8-9.
4 Emory 1963; Green 1966.
5 Grace 1964.
6 Ray 1926; Capell 1943; Capell 1962; Goodenough 1962.
7 Keesing 1967b.
8 Kwaio terms are reproduced in phonemic orthography, full details of which will be published in a forthcoming dictionary. Here the reader need only know the following phonological patterns and orthographic conventions:
(1) /x/ is used for an unvoiced velar spirant (X). (2) /I/ has two positional variants: (I) preceding /a/, /e/, and /o/ (r) preceding /i/ and /u/ (3) /b/, /d/, /g/ are prenasalised intervocalically and slightly prenasalised in initial position. (4) /xw/, /gw/, and /ŋw/ are single labialised phonemes. (5) Initial vowels are doubled in utterance-initial position or following some kinds of non-final juncture. English glosses of sociologically important Kwaio categories, and constructions involving tabooing, are enclosed in single quotes. (6) /?/ indicates a glottal stop. 9 Successes are made possible partly by ascription (descent status, seniority in a sibling set) but are always due to personal achievement as well.
10 These processes and relationships are documented in greater detail in Keesing 1969b.
11 c.f. Haas 1951.
12 c.f. Fox on San Cristobal adoption (1924).
13 The term walafu-na is used for great-great-grandparents and all further generation removals.
14 Keesing 1967a;1968b.
15 Ray 1926:481.
16 Conklin 1962.
17 See Conklin 1962.
18 Conklin 1962:121-122.
19 The latter two derived from the geographical divisions introduced by the government in the 1920s for taxation purposes.
20 Keesing 1968.
21 A point I argued strongly at a meeting of Oceanic anthropologists (Davenport, Grace, Green, and Oliver) regarding Melanesian culture history held in Santa Cruz, California, in July, 1967.
22 Deck 1933:34.
23 As, for instance in Mota (Codrington 1885:255).
24 Dyen 1963.
25 1962:176 ff.
|
||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||